Chapter 10: Disgust

Core Disgust

Core disgust is the original evolutionary defense caused by bad tasting and physically harmful objects that enter the mouth.  Core disgust is “the revulsion at prospect of oral incorporation of an offensive object” (Rozin & Fallon, 1987, p. 23).  Let’s break down this definition.  Oral incorporation means the offensive object is consumed through the mouth.  In fact, they believe that disgust first developed as a defense against eating deadly foods or eating foods that could be contaminated by other animals and body products.  With the word “prospect”, they highlight that core disgust may elicited by actually placing an offensive object in the mouth OR by thinking about an offensive object being placed in one’s mouth.  An offensive object is any object that is contaminated or has touched a contaminated object.  Core disgust is elicited by objects that are distasteful AND dangerous.  This means we do not experience core disgust when we eat something that tastes bad or feels funny but is not dangerous.  For example, some people HATE the taste of cilantro, but eating cilantro will not harm them.  My son refuses to eat apple sauce because of the texture – again not core disgust!  Second, core disgust is not just harm to the body.  Threats to physical harm without a bad taste would most likely elicit fear or anger.   Third, core disgust should be a universal emotion.  Thus, any cultural differences in food that elicit distaste would not be considered core disgust.  For instance, when studying abroad in Paris I was served a crab in a champagne glass!  When receiving the champagne glass I was definitely surprised and “grossed out,” but this disgust is caused by cultural expectations of food appropriateness, not by potential physical harm.

 

Core Disgust = Distasteful + Dangerous 

 

Cognitive appraisals that occur with core disgust represent our beliefs about the origin of food or the objects that food has touched.  It is important to keep in mind these appraisals are often automatic and sometimes inaccurate.  Three cognitive appraisals cause the emotion core disgust: 1) Oral Incorporation into the Self, 2) Offensiveness and 3) Contamination.  Let’s discuss each of these appraisals.

 

Threat of Oral Incorporation in the Self

This appraisal occurs because we perceive an offensive entity as entering the mouth to be more disgusting and harmful than an offensive entity entering though other parts of the body (i.e., skin, eyes, nose).  Thus, we will experience the greatest aversion to offensive objects entering the mouth than to other parts of the body.  Why? Well researchers believe the mouth is a boundary between the self and objects outside the self. This means, anything offensive we consume through the mouth could be incorporated or become part of the self.  In essence, the appraisal is “you are what you eat” and represents the perception that we take on the offensive and immoral properties of any offensive objects we eat.  Thus, when we consume an offensive object, we make an internal attribution that we are now disgusting.  Similarly, when we view others consuming offensive objects we make an external attribution – and conclude they are disgusting and offensive people. How would this play out in real life?  Well, in Madagascar a law prevented soldiers from eating hedgehogs because the government feared if soldiers ate hedgehogs the soldiers would exhibit the traits of the hedgehog!  What are traits of a hedgehog?  Well, hedgehogs tend to become timid and hide when they are in danger – qualities that we would not want in soldiers who defend! Other historical examples reference that in wartime, soldiers would eat the flesh of superior armies to take on the personality traits of “strength and courage.” The Aztecs are one group who engaged in exocannibalism, which means to consume the flesh of humans who are not included in one’s close social group. As if we don’t have enough examples, the Chijon gang, a South Korean gang, would eat the flesh of wealthy individuals to gain a higher status for the self.

 

Oral Incorporation: Food + Person = Person Takes on Traits of Food 

 

The appraisal “you are what you eat” was tested in a research study (Nemeroff & Rozin, 1989).  First, the authors determined the traits college students attributed to four types of living beings (boars, turtles, elephants, and plants).  Table 1 shows the lists people attribute to each group.  Then using these traits, participants read stories about individuals who ate these beings and rated the personality traits of these eaters. In Study 1, participants read stories about boar-eaters OR turtle-eaters and in study 2 about elephant-eaters OR plant-eaters.  Nemeroff and Rozin (1986) concluded that participants viewed boar-eaters to be higher on boar-like traits and turtle-eaters to be higher on turtle-like traits.  Similarly, participants viewed elephant-eaters to be higher on elephant-like traits and plant-eaters to be higher on plant-like traits.  Yet, these findings were very weak and further research should be conducted to assess the appraisal “you are what you eat.”

 

Table 1
College Students’ Perceptions of Traits Assigned to Four Living Beings

a table showing perception of traits assigned to Boar-eaters
Boar-eaters
Ungenerous
Irritable
Excitable
Loud, outspoken
Unreliable
Short-lived
Uncontrolled
Fast-moving
Good runners
Bearded
Heavyset
Aggressive
Brown Eyes
a table showing perception of traits assigned to Turtle-eaters
Turtle-eaters
Generous
Good-natured
Phlegmatic
Quiet and Shy
Reliable
Long-lived
Restrained
Slow-moving
Good swimmers
No facial hair
Slender
Peaceful
Green Eyes
a table showing perception of traits assigned to Elephant-eaters
Elephant-eaters
Interesting
Loud
Smart
Sociable
Aggressive
a table showing perception of traits assigned to Plant-eaters
Plant-eaters
Boring
Quiet
Not too smart
Unsociable
Peaceful

Note. These traits should be viewed as dimensional poles. Reproduced from ““You are what you eat”: Applying the demand‐free “impressions” technique to an unacknowledged belief,” by C. Nemeroff, and P. Rozin, 1989, Ethos, 17(1), pp. 60, 62 (https://doi.org/10.1525/eth.1989.17.1.02a00030). Copyright 1989 by Wiley.

 

Offensiveness

Offensiveness is an appraisal that occurs when we view aversion to eating animals or to eating the bodily products of any animal, including humans (Rozin et al., 2008).  Examples of body products include feces, urine, blood, and vomit.  The offensiveness appraisal occurs when people think about eating most animals and the body products of animals.  Again, this emphasizes that we do not need to actually eat the animal or body products, but that we experience core disgust when we think about doing so.  Researchers point out that offensiveness is supported by the fact that most cultures eat a small subset of animals and that cultures have taboos on the type of food to eat.  Some food taboos:

 

  • Avoiding foods with slimy textures that remind the self of slugs, worms, etc .
  • Avoiding foods that may have touched contaminated animals or humans (e.g., cockroaches, flies, vultures).
  • Avoiding animals that eat other animals (i.e., carnivores),
  • Avoiding animals that share intimate relationships with humans (i.e., dogs, cats)
  • Avoiding physically harmful animals (e.g., snakes, spiders) that elicit both fear and disgust.

 

When eating other animals, cultures even utilize certain methods to prevent the appraisal of offensiveness.  This includes changing the names for animal foods (e.g., ham, steak, pork instead of pig, cow, pig). In addition, we plate or present the food in a more appealing way and remove certain body parts like skin and heads.  These methods may be attempts for humans to reduce core disgust reactions by avoiding thoughts about the true animal they are consuming.  For example, look at the two photos below.  Which would you be more likely to eat?  Which elicits more disgust?  The first would elicit thoughts of offensiveness in many people and cause core disgust.  I say in many people because some people are very low in disgust sensitivity.  Disgust sensitivity is a personality trait which suggests some people experience disgust to a lot of different objects (high disgust sensitivity), whereas people low in disgust sensitivity are rarely disgusted.

 

A picture of a full cooked baby pig

Reproduced from “Spitfyre’s 50th birthday” by Nicole Bratt, 2012. Open Access, Creative Commons: Attribution-ShareAlike 2.0 Generic (CC BY-SA 2.0)Retrieved from this source.

 

Pork Tenderloin
Reproduced from “Pork tenderloin dish” by User:M, 2016. Open Access,  Creative Commons Attribution-Share Alike 4.0 International. Retrieved from: https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Pork_tenderloin_dish.png

 

Contamination

Contamination is the third appraisal of core disgust.  A contamination response occurs when we reject contaminated food from the mouth or simply avoid foods that touched a contaminated object, even if the contact occurred over a short period of time.  For example, refusing to drink a glass of milk after you remove a fly from the milk.  The fly only contacted the milk for a millisecond, but it is still possible the fly touched something contaminated.  Some believe the contamination response is an adaptation that functions to help humans avoid diseases and death.   Two appraisals explain the universal contamination response: 1) law of contagion and 2) law of similarity (Rozin et al., 1986).  The law of contagion occurs when people think “once in contact, always in contact.” In other words, the law of contagion assumes that when one neutral object touches a contaminated object, the properties of the contaminated object are permanently transferred to the neutral object, even though a brief contact between two objects may not actually result in contamination.  The contaminated object could be food, animal, or even an immoral person.  When we perceived that a neutral object that we put into our mouth has retained properties of a contaminated object, that causes core disgust.  Though rules and rituals, some cultures try to make the law of contagion less salient or conscious.  For instance, Judaism has kosher rules which for example require that different pots/pans be used for meat and dairy, so that the meat from the animal is not cooked in the animal’s own dairy.  In some Indian and Asian cultures, people eat with their right hands and use their left hands for contaminated activities (e.g., wiping their bottom, cleaning their feet).   Do we have any rituals in North American that reduce thoughts about contamination?  What about the five-second rule? In general, people may avoid this appraisal by not thinking about the people making their food or thinking about the animal that the food came from.    The law of similarity states that if two products appear similar, then the two products have the same underlying properties as well.  Said another way the appearance of an object represents reality; the image equals the object.  This applies to disgust such that an object that looks like something disgusting must be disgusting itself.  For instance, people refuse to eat objects that look like feces, such as melted chocolate.  Together, the laws of contagion and similarity are called the laws of sympathetic magic – magic because long ago, remnants of an individual (e.g., hair, fingernail scrapings) were thought to represent the spirit of the whole individual.

Rozin et al. (1986) tested the laws of contagion and similarity.  To test for the law of contagion, participants tasted and rated their liking for juice – these ratings represented the pre-test.  Then, participants watched while the experimenter placed one of two objects into the cup of juice – 1) plastic birthday candle (control) and  2) a dried and dead sterilized cockroach (experimental).  Participants were all told that the dead cockroach had been sterilized and was safe. After removing the candle or roach from the juice, participants rated their liking for the juice on a 200 mm scale.  Figure 1 shows that participants reported significantly LESS liking for the juice when contaminated by a sterile cockroach compared to the pretest trial.

 

Figure 1
Law Contagion Findings (Rozin et al., 1986)
Two bar graphs. There is a pre-test and contagion bar for both the roach graph; and the candle graph. The y axis measures the liking rating on a 200 mm line, which starts at 0, increases in increments of 20, to a maximum of 200. Roach Pre-test bar - 120. Roach contagion bar - 15. Candle pre-test bar - 105. Candle Contagion bar - 101.
Reproduced from “Operation of The Laws of Sympathetic Magic in Disgust and Other Domains,” by P. Rozin, L. Millman, and C. Nemeroff, 1986, . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(4), p. 706, (https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.4.703). Copyright 1986 by the American Psychological Association.

 

To test the law of similarity, participants completed two other tasks. In the first fudge task, participants were given a square piece of chocolate fudge and allowed to eat the fudge. After this, participants were given the same fudge – except now the fudge was either in the shape of a muffin/disc or in the shape of dog feces. In the second rubber task, participant were given two rubber objects that they were told were clean. These two objects were a rubber sink stopper and rubber imitation vomit. Participants rated how much they would like to hold each object in between their lips. Results showed that participants liked the fudge in the shape of a disc/muffin significantly more than the fudge in the shape of the dog feces Figure 2. Also, participants preferred to place the rubber stopper in their mouths than the rubber vomit – even though the stopper and vomit were made of the same exact rubber!

 

Figure 2
Law of Similarity Findings (Rozin et al., 1986)
Two Bar Graphs. One graph for the fudge task, one graph for the rubber task. Each graph has 2 bars (4 bars total). The y axis measures the liking rating on a 200 mm line, it starts at 0, increases in increments of 20, to a maximum of 200. Fudge task, dog feces bar - 95. Fudge task, Disc / Muffin bar - 140. Rubber task, rubber vomit bar - 20. Rubber task, rubber stopper bar - 70.
Reproduced from “Operation of The Laws of Sympathetic Magic in Disgust and Other Domains,” by P. Rozin, L. Millman, and C. Nemeroff, 1986, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(4), p. 707, (https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.4.703). Copyright 1986 by the American Psychological Association.

License

Share This Book