Chapter 5: Dimensional Models

Watson and Tellegen’s (1985) Circumplex Model

Directions: For the below emotion words, circle how much you currently feel this emotion.  Then, average your total positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA).  To average your scores, add up all your answers for positive affect (PA) and divide by 10.  Follow the same pattern for negative affect (NA).  Averages should range between 1 and 5.  This scale is called the Positive Affect Negative Affect Scale-Momentary (PANAS-Momentary; Watson et al., 1988) 

 

A graphic meant to score numerous emotions on a scale of 1 to 5. 1 being very slightly / not at all, 5 being extremely. The emotions listed are: Interested, excited, strong, enthusiastic, proud, alert, inspired, determined, attentive, active. There is also an entire row at the bottom labeled: Average PA score.

A graphic meant to score numerous emotions on a scale of 1 to 5. 1 being very slightly / not at all, 5 being extremely. The emotions listed are: Distressed, upset, guilty, scared, hostile, irritable, ashamed, nervous, jittery, and afraid. There is also an entire row at the bottom labeled: Average PA score.

 

An image displaying a picture of Watson on the left, and Tellegen on the right.
A picture of Watson and Tellegen

 

 

Watson and Tellegen (1985) conducted similar studies to Russell and Barrett (1999).  Their findings suggested that the lines identified by Russell and Barrett were not placed through the greatest cluster of points.  So, Watson and Tellegen (1985) rotated Russell’s (1980) circumplex 45 degrees (see below) and drew two new dimensions through the greatest cluster of points, which they labeled High Positive Affect and High Negative Affect.

 

Figure 7

Watson and Tellegen’s (1985) 45° Rotation of Russell’s (1980) Circumplex

 

Watson and Tellegen’s (1985) Circumplex Model
Reproduced from “Toward a consensual structure of mood.,” by D.T. Watson and A. Tellegen, 1985, Psychological Bulletin, 98(2), p. 221 (https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.219). Copyright 1985 by the American Psychological Association.

 

 

Rotating Russell’s (1980) circumplex resulted in eight octants subsumed by four dimensions: pleasantness, engagement, positive affect, and negative affect.  Pleasantness/unpleasantness and engagement/disengagement are bipolar measures, whereas positive affect and negative affect are unipolar dimensions (although Watson and Tellegen state they are bipolar dimensions as well).  Pleasantness ranges from moderately arousing pleasant emotions to moderately arousing negative emotions.  They are moderately arousing emotions because these emotions are located between strong engagement and disengagement.  The engagement dimension measures high to low arousing emotions that are moderately pleasant or unpleasant.  High Negative Affect (NA) and High Positive Affect (PA) represent highly activated positive and negative emotions because PA and NA are located closest to engagement.  Specifically, High NA is feeling unpleasant, highly arousing emotions.  High PA is feeling pleasant and highly activated emotions.   Later, Positive Affect and Negative Affect were renamed Positive Activation and Negative Activation to emphasize the terms represent highly activating emotions.

Relationships between emotions are determined by how close in space the octants of each emotion are.  Table 1 explains how to interpret the correlational relationships based on octant location(s).

 

Table 1

Correlational Relationship between Emotion Words based on Octant Location

A table showing the correlational relationship between emotion words based on Octant Location
Octant Location of Emotion Words Relationship
Emotions words located in same octant Strongly, positively correlated
Emotion words in adjacent octant Moderately, positively correlated
Emotion Words 90°apart Uncorrelated
Emotion Words 180° apart Strongly, Negatively correlated

 

 

Watson and Tellegen (1985) found that self-reports of emotions clustered mostly around High PA and High NA.  Thus, they concluded that PA and NA are the only two dimensions of the model and are most representative of true emotions.  So, they believe the important emotions are all high in arousal.   Because PA and NA are 90 degrees from each other, PA and NA are uncorrelated with each other.  This means an individual could experience High PA and High NA at the same time.  In fact, Watson and Tellegen (1985) suggest that mixed emotions occur in the octants between High PA and High NA.  It is important to note that this model does not allow happiness and sadness to co-occur.

 

So, what about the low poles of PA and NA?  The low poles represent the absence of PA and NA.  Low NA would be the absence of highly arousing negative emotions and Low PA would be the absence of highly arousing positive emotions.  So, low NA represents pleasant emotions that are low in arousal.  This is because low NA is closest in space to the pleasantness and disengagement dimensions.  Similarly, low PA would be the presence of positive deactivation. So, low PA represents unpleasant emotions that are low in arousal.  This is because low PA is closest in space to unpleasantness and disengagement.

 

In this model, PA and NA are usually negatively correlated, such that an increase in negative affect should decrease positive affect.  But these dimensions may also be uncorrelated or positively correlated.  It is important to point out that researchers disagree over whether PA and NA are unipolar or bipolar.  Watson and Tellegen (1985) believe that PA and NA are bipolar – because an increase in high PA results in a decrease of unpleasant, low arousal emotions.  Yet, some researchers suggest that PA and NA are unipolar.  For instance, PA could range from the presence of highly activating positive emotions (e.g., excited, elated) to the absence of highly active positive emotions (e.g., dull, sluggish).

 

PA and NA are measured using the PANAS-Momentary (Watson et al., 1988), which you completed earlier.  One criticism of this theory is that some of the emotion words for high PA and NA seem like motivational states (e.g., attentive, alert), while others seem like personality traits (e.g., determined, hostile) and still others like moods (e.g., irritable).

License

Share This Book